Incompetent People Really Have No Clue

From: upgrade (fm1@amug.org)
Date: Fri Feb 04 2000 - 11:12:33 MST


The enclosed article seems to provide support for my
"Unreality Imperative" theory
<http://www.buildfreedom.com/psychology/unreality_imperative.html>.

Frederick Mann

Incompetent People Really Have No Clue, Studies Find They're
blind to own failings, others' skills

Erica Goode, New York Times

Tuesday, January 18, 2000 ©2000 San Francisco Chronicle

There are many incompetent people in the world. Dr. David A.
Dunning is haunted by the fear that he might be one of them.
Dunning, a professor of psychology at Cornell, worries about this
because, according to his research, most incompetent people
do not know that they are incompetent.

On the contrary. People who do things badly, Dunning has found
in studies conducted with a graduate student, Justin Kruger, are
usually supremely confident of their abilities -- more confident, in
fact, than people who do things well.

``I began to think that there were probably lots of things that I
was bad at, and I didn't know it,'' Dunning said.

One reason that the ignorant also tend to be the blissfully
self-assured, the researchers believe, is that the skills required
for competence often are the same skills necessary to recognize
competence.

The incompetent, therefore, suffer doubly, they suggested in a
paper appearing in the December issue of the Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology.

``Not only do they reach erroneous conclusions and make unfortunate
choices, but their incompetence robs them of the ability to realize
it,'' wrote Kruger, now an assistant professor at the University of
Illinois, and Dunning.

This deficiency in ``self-monitoring skills,'' the researchers said,
helps explain the tendency of the humor-impaired to persist in
telling jokes that are not funny, of day traders to repeatedly jump
into the market -- and repeatedly lose out -- and of the politically
clueless to continue holding forth at dinner parties on the fine
points of campaign strategy.

In a series of studies, Kruger and Dunning tested their theory of
incompetence. They found that subjects who scored in the lowest
quartile on tests of logic, English grammar and humor were also the
most likely to ``grossly overestimate'' how well they had performed.
In all three tests, subjects' ratings of their ability were positively
linked to their actual scores. But the lowest-ranked participants
showed much greater distortions in their self-estimates.

Asked to evaluate their performance on the test of logical
reasoning, for example, subjects who scored only in the 12th
percentile guessed that they had scored in the 62nd percentile,
and deemed their overall skill at logical reasoning to be at the 68th
percentile.

Similarly, subjects who scored at the 10th percentile on the
grammar test ranked themselves at the 67th percentile in the
ability to ``identify grammatically correct standard English,''
and estimated their test scores to be at the 61st percentile.

On the humor test, in which participants were asked to rate
jokes according to their funniness (subjects' ratings were
matched against those of an ``expert'' panel of professional
comedians), low-scoring subjects were also more apt to have
an inflated perception of their skill. But because humor is
idiosyncratically defined, the researchers said, the results
were less conclusive.

Unlike unskilled counterparts, the most able subjects in the
study, Kruger and Dunning found, were likely to underestimate
their competence. The researchers attributed this to the fact
that, in the absence of information about how others were doing,
highly competent subjects assumed that others were performing
as well as they were -- a phenomenon psychologists term the
``false consensus effect.''

When high-scoring subjects were asked to ``grade'' the
grammar tests of their peers, however, they quickly revised
their evaluations of their own performance. In contrast, the
self-assessments of those who scored badly themselves were
unaffected by the experience of grading others; some subjects
even further inflated their estimates of their own abilities.

``Incompetent individuals were less able to recognize competence
in others,'' the researchers concluded.

In a final experiment, Dunning and Kruger set out to discover if
training would help modify the exaggerated self-perceptions of
incapable subjects. In fact, a short training session in logical
reasoning did improve the ability of low-scoring subjects to
assess their performance realistically, they found.

The findings, the psychologists said, support Thomas Jefferson's
assertion that ``he who knows best knows how little he knows.''
And the research meshes neatly with other work indicating that
overconfidence is common; studies have found, for example, that
the vast majority of people rate themselves as ``above average''
on a wide array of abilities -- though such an abundance of talent
would be impossible in statistical terms. This overestimation,
studies indicate, is more likely for tasks that are difficult than
for those that are easy.

Such studies are not without critics. Dr. David C. Funder, a
psychology professor at the University of California at Riverside,
for example, said he suspects that most lay people have only
a vague idea of the meaning of ``average'' in statistical terms.

``I'm not sure the average person thinks of `average' or
`percentile' in quite that literal a sense,'' Funder said, ``so
`above average' might mean to them `pretty good,' or `OK,'
or `doing all right.' And if, in fact, people mean something
subjective when they use the word, then it's really hard to
evaluate whether they're right or wrong, using the statistical
criterion.''

But Dunning said his current research and past studies
indicated there are many reasons why people would tend
to overestimate their competency and not be aware of it.

In various situations, feedback is absent, or at least ambiguous;
even a humorless joke, for example, is likely to be met with
polite laughter. And faced with incompetence, social norms
prevent most people from blurting out ``You stink!'' -- truthful
though this assessment may be.

URL:
http://www.sfgate.com/cgi-bin/article.cgi?file=/chronicle/archive/2000/01/18
/MN73840.DTL



This archive was generated by hypermail 2b29 : Thu Jul 27 2000 - 14:03:24 MDT